pmc logo imageJournal ListSearchpmc logo image
Logo of wtpaUKPMC FundersUK PubMed CentralAbout Author ManuscriptsSubmit a Manuscript
J Fluency Disord.Author manuscript; available in PMC 2008 February 6.
Published in final edited form as:
PMCID: PMC2231599
UKMSID: UKMS1001
INTERNAL STRUCTURE OF CONTENT WORDS LEADING TO LIFESPAN DIFFERENCES IN PHONOLOGICAL DIFFICULTY IN STUTTERING
PETER HOWELL, JAMES AU-YEUNG, and STEVIE SACKIN
University College London
Address correspondence to Peter Howell, Department of Psychology, University College London, Gower Street, London WC1E 6BT, England.
Abstract
This study investigated whether frequency of stuttering was affected by factors that specify the phonological difficulty of a sound and whether and how any influences vary across age groups. Analyses were performed separately on content words and function words. The phonological factors examined were: a) Whether the word contained a late emerging consonant (LEC); and b) Whether the word contained a consonant string (CS). Analyses showed that these factors occurred at different rates across the age groups used (children under 12, teenagers between 12 and 18, and adults). A more detailed breakdown was also reported of frequency of usage of LEC and CS over age groups depending on whether and where these factors occurred in the content words; all nine combinations of no LEC, word-initial LEC, non-initial LEC with no CS, word-initial CS, and non-initial CS were examined. Usage of certain of these nine categories varied over age groups. Friedman statistic on the ratio of stuttering (proportion of stuttered words in a particular word class divided by the proportion of words in that particular word class) showed that the frequency of stuttering remained high for adult speakers when CS and LEC both occurred in a word and when they appeared in word-initial position. These findings support a recently proposed theory that accounts for life-span changes in stuttering.
Keywords: Lifespan, phonology, content
INTRODUCTION
Two recent papers addressed the issue whether measures of phonological difficulty can specify on what words people who stutter will experience difficulty (Throneburg, Yairi & Paden, 1994; Howell & Au-Yeung, 1995a). The factors examined in each of these studies were: a) Whether a word contained a late emerging consonant (LEC). The consonants that emerge late in development in English speaking children according to Sander (1972) are /r, l, s, z, dƺ, v, t∫, h, Θ, ð, ∫ , ƺ; b) Whether the word contained a consonant string (CS); and c) Whether the word was multisyllabic (MS). Speakers acquire LEC and CS at later ages than other phonological structures. LEC is defined on the basis when these consonants occur during phonological development of average young children. Consonant strings are acquired later than singletons (Smit, 1993; Wellman, et al., 1931) and they appear less often in frequently used words than singletons (Locke, 1988).
The position taken in this article is that late developing sounds are not only relevant to child phonological development, but may also indicate either the motoric difficulty or the pre-motor planning of different sounds for speakers of all ages. LEC and CS may be acquired late if the articulators require time to mature (see Schwartz, 1988, for a discussion on motor limitation and language development). However, these low-level (physical or biological) limitations are hard to be distinguished from high-level factors (planning or representation) in phonological development (c.f. Smith, 1988). The comment on historical sound change by Slobin (1977) also reflects the view that “forms whichare late to be acquired by children are presumably also relatively difficult for adults to process” (p. 194; cited in Locke, 1983). Adults have been reported to avoid words with consonant sequences that they find difficult (Locke, 1983; Schwartz, 1988). The current study tests if LEC and CS are particularly difficult for people who stutter and how this varies between different age ranges. Sander's (1972) study based on data from children up to eight years old and other studies on avoidance of words were conducted on children still undergoing phonological development. The current study uses age ranges up to 40. It is predicted that sounds which are difficult for young children will continue to be problematic for adults who stutter.
Both LEC and CS involve consonants and consonants are known to be related to high incidence of stuttering for people who stutter (Brown, 1945). Consequently, Throneburg et al. (1994) investigated whether words containing CS and LEC have high levels of motor difficulty that results in these sounds being stuttered in a group of children. Howell and Au-Yeung (1995a) investigated these factors in children who stutter over three age groups (2 to 6, 6 plus to 9 and 9 plus to 12). No dependence of frequency of stuttering on phonological difficulty was found by either Throneburg et al. (1994) or Howell and Au-Yeung (1995a).
In both these studies, each word was assessed in terms of whether it contained one or more of the three factors that indicate phonological difficulty. A limitation in these studies is that no distinction was made concerning the position where an LEC or CS occurred within a word. Consequently even for monosyllable words, CS and LEC factors can be distributed over different word positions in several ways: CSs can be word initial (e.g., ‘quick’), or non-initial (e.g., ‘found’); LEC can also be word initial (e.g., ‘like’) or non-initial (e.g., ‘fail’); A word containing CS and LEC can have both CS and LEC word initial (e.g., ‘tried’, or ‘school’), both CS and LEC can be non-initial (e.g., ‘helps’) or CS and LEC can be at different positions (e.g., ‘quiz’ has CS initial and LEC non-initial and ‘lent’ has these factors in the reverse positions).
Two observations emphasize the importance of performing the analyses with regard to the position that the phonological factors occupy in a word. First, stuttering almost always occurs on the first phones of a word (Brown, 1945; Wingate, 1982, 1988). Second, there may be some interaction between CS and LEC such that when they occur in the same word position, this enhances their difficulty. Thus, a word like “school” that has a CS and LEC that occur in word-initial position would be expected to cause more difficulty than “quiz” where the CS occurs before the vocalic segment and the LEC after it. Taken together, these two observations suggest that if CS and LEC both occur before the first vowel in a word, the word would be highly prone to being stuttered (what is referred to as focal difficulty). The experiment reported below set out to determine whether incidence of stuttering depends on CS and LEC positions and, in particular, whether words with focal difficulty have a higher chance of being stuttered than other CS/LEC combinations within a word.
Examination of these factors over age groups is also important because differential influences would be expected according to the speaker's age. There are two alternative views about how such influences vary with speaker's age. First, when speakers make their initial attempts at sounds with LEC and CS, they may have difficulty. Once they have acquired the ability to produce these sounds, they are no more difficult than others that do not involve these factors. In this case, CS and LEC would lead to production problems in early development but not at later ages.
The second view is that sounds that are difficult early in development continue to be motorically difficult for older speakers, particularly those who persist in their stuttering. Van Lieshout, et al. (1995) offer some support for this view. They investigated EMG activity in fluent adult speakers on words that contained linguistic factors known to affect frequency of stuttering. They found that words that are known to affect frequency of stuttering in adults resulted in increased EMG activity in their speakers. Our interpretation of these results is that sounds with a high likelihood of being stuttered are problematic even for fluent speakers into adulthood. This view is similar to one that Throneburg et al. (1994) discuss about how CS and LEC could give rise to motor difficulty. In this, they suggest that CS and LEC may reflect the difficulty-loading on the supposedly inferior peripheral articulatory system of children who stutter. The alternative viewpoint they raise is that these factors could cause difficulty in producing sounds containing them due to high level of demand on pre-motor planning. Though Van Lieshout et al. (1995) did not investigate CS and LEC, if sounds with these properties are acquired late because they are motorically difficult for speakers in general, it would be expected that they would continue to cause problems into adulthood as with words containing the factors van Lieshout et al. (1995) investigated.
At the outset, various sources of literatures were reviewed which showed that CS and LEC are acquired at different ages and that they would influence the motoric difficulty of sounds. Consideration of the influence of these phonological factors on stuttering has concentrated on motor difficulty rather than the differences in the usage of these sounds/sound sequences over the development of languages. All following analyses are conducted on spontaneous speech samples. This allows changes in usage over age groups to be established as well as to ascertain the effects motorically difficult sounds have on stuttering. In the first analyses, content words (nouns, main verbs, adverbs, and adjectives) are treated separately from function words (pronouns, articles, prepositions, conjunctions, and auxiliary verbs) because of the potentially different role dysfluencies on these word classes have on stuttering (Au-Yeung, Howell, & Pilgrim, 1998; Howell, Au-Yeung, & Sackin, 1999). To test between the views concerning whether the influences of CS and LEC are predominant in young or old age groups, the incidence of stuttering on content and function words is investigated in speakers who stutter that differ in age. A very low proportion of function words contains CS and LEC and when they do, this is not associated with stuttering. Consequently, function words are dropped from further analysis. The influence of position that CS or LEC occupy in a word is examined to check whether words are more likely to be stuttered when these factors occupy initial position in content words. The effect when both CS and LEC occur together in word-initial position (focal difficulty) is also examined.
METHOD
Subjects
All 51 participants were diagnosed as stuttering by a speech pathologist in a speech pathology clinic in their neighborhood. They were then seen by a second speech pathologist, who independently diagnosed them as person who stutters. The 51 speakers were recorded in spontaneous speech. They were divided into three age groups. There were 21 children in Group 1 aged between 3 and 11 (mean age 7.3 years with a SD of 1.8), 18 teenagers aged between 11 and 18 (Group 2, mean age 13.1 years with a SD of 2.5) and 12 adults aged 18 plus (Group 3, mean age 28.3 years with a SD of 6.7). The division at age 11 divides children who stutter predominantly on function words from those who shift to content words (Au-Yeung, Howell, & Pilgrim, 1998; Howell, Au-Yeung, & Sackin, 1999) and this age division is maintained so that comparison with these two studies can be made. The mean severity rates of stuttering of the speakers were similar and a graphical representation of their overall stuttering rate is given in Figure 1.
Figure 1Figure 1
Boxplots of stuttering severity of speakers in different age groups. The box for each age group indicates the inter-quartile range of stuttering rates and the mean is shown by the solid horizontal line within the box. The full range of stuttering rates (more ...)
Recording and Transcription
The spontaneous speech recordings were made in a relaxed atmosphere after speakers had completed some standard assessment recordings (these involved reading a set text and a period of dialogue interaction). Topics for conversation were suggested to the participants such as family, friends, television, and so on. These topics are suitable for all age groups. The recordings were made in a sound-treated booth. The recordings of the participants' speech were transcribed using a broad phonetic transcription in fluent regions and a narrow system in the region of dysfluencies. The transcription system uses upper case letter to represent dysfluencies and each represent 50 ms duration. For example, :B B “blaak represents a part word repetition while :SSSSSspeed represents a prolonged ‘s’ which is 250 ms longer than the average fluent ‘s’. A colon before the word signals a content word and a forward slash a function word. The double quote indicates a primary stressed syllable. Kadi-Hanifi and Howell (1992) provided a more detailed description of the transcription used.
Note that since the analyses were made on the basis of these phonetic transcriptions, if speakers simplify sounds (e.g. change an /r/ for a /w/) this can change the phonological designation of a word (from LEC to non LEC in this example). Prosodic features were marked in the transcriptions but these are not used in the current analysis.
Analysis
Inter-judge reliability measures were obtained by asking a second transcriber to re-transcribe speech materials from eight speakers chosen at random. The agreement between the two judges on content/function word was 98% giving a kappa coefficient of 0.96 which is much higher than chance (Fleiss, 1971). All words were classified as to whether they contained a stuttering. Inter-judge agreement of fluency was 96% on all words with a kappa coefficient of 0.92.
Word-based analyses were performed as follows: First each content word (or function word) was classified with respect to whether it had one or more of the factors LEC and CS. The position where LEC and CS occurred in a word was also determined. Word-initial position was defined with respect to whether the CS or LEC occurred on the consonants before the first vowel. All remaining word positions were considered non-initial.
RESULTS
Analysis 1: Content Word (Single factor)
The frequency of occurrence of content words possessing the CS and LEC phonological factors was examined first to establish any lifespan trends.
LEC
For this analysis, the percentage of content words not containing an LEC (LECx), the percentage of content words containing an LEC in word-initial position (LECi) and content words containing an LEC in non-initial word position (LECn) were calculated. A one-way ANOVA was carried out across age groups for each word category, LECx, LECi, and LECn. Significant differences across age groups occurred for all three categories of words. For LECx, F(2,48) = 13.35, p < 0.001; for LECi, F(2,48) = 5 5.91, p < 0.01; and for LECn, F(2,48) = 7.06, p < 0.005. Post hoc Tukey test showed that as age increased the usage of words without LEC (LECx) decreased significantly. Speakers in group 3 used considerably more LECi and LECn words than speakers in group 1. The mean percentages of usage of the three classes of words for each age group are presented in Figure 2.
Figure 2Figure 2
Mean percentages of word usage of content words without late emerging consonant (LEC) and with LEC at word initial and non-initial positions.
Next, we examined whether stuttering occurred differentially over LEC word classes. For each speaker, the percentage of stuttered content words found in each of the three LEC classifications was divided by the corresponding percentage of usage calculated above to give a ratio measure. The mean ratios for individual word class and speaker group are shown in Figure 3. A ratio of 1.0 is expected if all the words in different categories are stuttered equally (the percentage of stuttered words in all stuttered words equals to the percentage of that particular category of word out of all words used). The three ratios were ranked for each individual speaker and a Friedman test was carried out for each age group. Significant result was only obtained for speakers in group 1, Fr = 6.32, DF = 2, N = 19, p < 0.05 (adjusted for ties, only 19 speakers out of 21 contributed to the analysis as the other two speakers did not have enough stuttering or content words). Post hoc analyses with sign tests were carried out on all the combinations and the result indicated that speakers in group 1 stuttered significantly more on LECi content words than LECx content words.
Figure 3Figure 3
Mean stuttering ratios of content words without late emerging consonant (LEC) and with LEC at word initial and non-initial positions.
CS
Analyses similar to the ones carried out for the LEC factor were carried out on the content words but with words classified as having no CS (CSx), having CS at word initial position (CSi) and having CS at non word initial position (CSn). One-way ANOVAs separately for each of the three word categories with age group as factor, showed that only CSn words were used at different rates across age groups (F(2,48) = 3.29, p , 0.05). Post hoc Tukey analysis showed that group 1 speakers used less CSn words than speakers in group 3. Mean percentages of the usage of the different categories of words for each speaker group are shown in Figure 4.
Figure 4Figure 4
Mean percentages of word usage of content words without consonant string (CS) and with CS at word initial and non-initial positions.
Influence of CS class on stuttering was examined next. The percentage of content words that were stuttered for each of the three CS classes (CSx, CSi, and CSn) was divided by the corresponding percentage of usage calculated above to give a similar ratio to that in the analysis of the LEC factor. Within each age group, a Friedman test was carried out on the ranking of the ratios across the three word categories (ranked within each individual speaker). Significant differences were obtained in group 1 (Fr = 8.00, DF = 2, N = 19, p < 0.05), group 2 (Fr = 6.17, DF = 2, N = 12, p < 0.05; only 12 out of the 18 speakers contributed enough speech/stuttering under this word classification), and was almost significant in group 3 (Fr = 5.91, DF = 2, N = 12, p = 0.052). Post hoc analyses with sign tests showed that all speaker groups stuttered more on CSi words than CSx words. The mean ratios for individual word class and speaker group are given in Figure 5.
Figure 5Figure 5
Mean stuttering ratios of content words without consonant string (CS) and with CS at word initial and non-initial positions.
Analysis 2: Function Word (Single factor)
The same analyses carried out on content words in Analysis 1 were carried out on function words.
LEC
Analyses similar to the ones carried out for the LEC factor on content words were carried out on the function words first with all words classified as having no LEC (LECx), having LEC at word initial position (LECi) and having LEC at non word initial position (LECn). Separate ANOVAs for each of the three word categories over age groups showed that only LECn words differed (F(2,48) = 6.69, p < 0.005). Post hoc Tukey analysis showed that adult speakers in group 3 used more LECn words than younger speakers in group 1 and those in group 2. The mean percentages of usage of different category of words for each speaker group are given in Figure 6.
Figure 6Figure 6
Mean percentages of word usage of function words without late emerging consonant (LEC) and with LEC at word initial and non-initial positions.
Whether stuttering on function words depended on LEC class was examined. The percentage of function words that were stuttered for each of the three LEC classes (LECx, LECi, and LECn) was divided by the corresponding percentage of usage which gives a similar ratio measure to that obtained for the LEC factor on content words. For each speaker, the three ratios were ranked. For each age group, a Friedman test for the rankings was carried out. No significant differences occurred.
CS
Analyses similar to the ones carried out for the LEC factor on function words were carried out with the function words classified as having no CS (CSx), having CS at word initial position (CSi) and having CS at non-word initial position (CSn). A one-way ANOVA was carried out for each of the three word categories separately across age groups. Only CSn words differed significantly in usage across age groups (F(2,48) = 9.53, p < 0.001). Post hoc Tukey analysis showed that group 3 speakers used fewer CSn words than speakers in group 1. The mean percentages of usage of different category of words for each speaker group are given in Figure 7.
Figure 7Figure 7
Mean percentages of word usage of function words without consonant string (CS) and with CS at word initial and non-initial positions.
It is clear from Figure 7, the usage of function words that have CS at word initial position is miniscule and cannot support any meaningful analysis. Therefore, this category of words was left out from the analysis of stuttering ratio. Only the proportions of stuttering of CSx and CSn were investigated. Paired sign tests were carried out for each age group but no significant differences occurred across the two CS word categories.
Analysis 3: Focal vs Distributed CS and LEC Factors
From Analysis 1, CS was found to lead to speaking difficulties when they were in initial position in content words for all speaker groups while LEC at word initial position caused problems for the youngest speakers (group 1). Collapsing together LEC over different CS positions and CS over different LEC positions disguises whether these factors interact when they occupy common positions. Consequently, the way frequency of usage changes when LEC are examined jointly with CS was explored. Words were classified into nine categories according to whether the word had no CS (CSx), had CS at word-initial position (CSi) or CS at non-word initial position (CSn) crossed with whether the word had no LEC (LECx), had LEC before the first vowel (LECi) or had LEC after the first vowel (LECn). The nine orthogonal categories were, then, (1) CSx-LECx, (2) CSx-LECi, (3) CSx-LECn, (4) CSi-LECx, (5) CSi-LECi, (6) CSi-LECn, (7) CSn-LECx, (8) CSn-LECi, and (9) CSn-LECn. Category (5), CSi-LECi, has “focal” difficulty as defined in the introduction (this category contains an LEC and a CS and both are in word initial position). Such analysis was not carried out for the function words as it is clear from Analysis 2 that the usage of function words with CS at initial position (CSi) was very low and the low occurrence would be exacerbated if the categories were subdivided.
Usage
The mean percentages of usage of the nine word categories are shown in Figure 8. The data for category (1) show that as age increases, speakers use fewer content words containing neither LEC nor CS (this drops from 37.11% in the child age group to 24.15% in the adults, roughly a 13% decrease). In all other categories, except focally difficult sounds (CSi-LECi), more words were used within the word category as age increased (CSi-LECi showed a small decrease). A one-way ANOVA with the factor age group (three levels) was carried out for each of the nine categories. Post hoc Tukey analyses were carried out when the results were significant (p < 0.05). For CSx-LECx words, there was a significant decrease over age groups (F(2,48) = 17.42, p < 0.001). For CSx-LECn, CSi-LECx, and CSn-LECi words, the speakers in group 2 and group 3 used more words in these categories than speakers in group 1 (respectively, F(2,48) = 8.76, p < 0.005; F(2,48) = 3.91, p < 0.05; F(2,48) = 6.59; p < 0.005).
Figure 8Figure 8
Mean percentages of word usage of content words without consonant string (CSx) and late emerging consonant (LECx) and with CS and LEC at word initial (CSi, LECi) and non-initial positions (CSn, LECn).
Stuttering Ratio
Any of the nine categories which were used less than 5% when averaged over age groups (Figure 8) were dropped from further analyses. The word categories that were excluded from statistical analysis by this criterion were: (4) CSi-LECx, (6) CSi-LECn, (7) CSn-LECx, and (8) CSn-LECi. The ratio of stuttered to all words used in the category were calculated for the remaining categories in a similar fashion as for the three grosser categories in Analysis 1. The remaining five ratios were ranked within each speaker. A Friedman test on the rankings was carried out for each age group. Significant results were obtained for speakers in group 2, Fr = 13.24, DF = 4, N = 12, p < 0.05 (adjusted for ties), and those in group 3, Fr = 12.80, DF = 4, N = 12, p < 0.005. Post hoc analyses with sign tests showed that for speakers in group 2, the stuttering on the focally difficult word category CSi-LECi and the category CSx-LECi was significantly higher than those for words classified as CSx-LECx and CSx-LECn. For speakers in group 3, the focally difficult word category (CSi-LECi) was stuttered significantly more than the words in categories CSx-LECx, CSx-LECi, and CSx-LECn. The ratio for CSn-LECn is also significantly higher than that for CSx-LECx. The mean stuttering ratios of the five word categories are shown in Figure 9.
Figure 9Figure 9
Mean stuttering ratios of content words without consonant string (CSx) and late emerging consonant (LECx) and with CS and LEC at word initial (CSi, LECi) and non-initial positions (CSn, LECn).
DISCUSSION
The data on frequency of usage of LEC and CS separately (regardless of word position and whether the other factor occurred or not) showed that there was a decline in usage of content words without an LEC (Figure 2) and without a CS (Figure 4) over age groups. These words may be regarded as phonologically simple. Correspondingly, there is evidence of an increase in usage of content words containing LEC (in both word-initial and non-initial positions) and similar non-significant trends for CS. These observations suggest that as usage of simple words declines, usage of phonologically more complex words increases. These findings on frequency of usage are consistent with what would be expected assuming that simpler phonological structures are used more frequently by younger speakers. Dobrich and Scarborough (1992) found that young children used words selectively according to the phonological features in the words.
The more detailed breakdown of both CS and LEC by word position showed that words containing neither of these factors declined in usage over age groups. Once again, this suggests that simple phonological structures are used less frequently as speakers get older. The decrease in usage of this category over age groups is dramatic. The reduction due to the decline in content words with neither an LEC nor a CS is offset by increases in words with more complex phonological structures. It seems likely that this reflects changes in vocabulary which would be expected to occur over the entire lifespan.
Previous studies carried out by Throneburg et al. (1994) and by Howell and Au-Yeung (1995a) found no effect of either LEC or CS on the occurrence of stuttering. The current study, however, showed that the phonological factors of LEC and CS affected frequency of stuttering when word positions of these factors were considered in Analysis 1. Speakers from all age groups stuttered significantly more on content words that starting with CSs than those that contained no CS at all. Moreover, the youngest speakers (group 1) stuttered significantly more on words starting with LECs. On the other hand, Analysis 2 showed that no effect of the two phonological factors, LEC and CS, was found on the stuttering of function words. This is compatible with the suggestion put forward by Au-Yeung and Howell (1998) that word external properties trigger stuttering on function words. At the same time, word internal factors, in this case LEC and CS, affected stuttering in all speakers. This suggests that content words and function words need to be treated differently when studying their linguistic effect on stuttering.
When the interaction of CS and LEC was considered in Analysis 3, variation in stuttering ratio was investigated over the five LEC/CS classes where there were sufficient occurrences (categories 1, 2, 3, 5, and 9). Significant differences over word categories occurred for speakers in age group 2 and group 3. When content words contained neither an LEC nor a CS (category 1), the words were stuttered less and can be regarded as phonologically easy. Similarly category 3 can be considered easy since many of this class of content words include regularly used plural word endings like “-s.” The category of words with focal difficulty, on the other hand, showed an increased chance of being stuttered.
In normal phonological development, LECs are used correctly much earlier than CSs by the average child. Sander (1972) found that the age of customary articulation for LECs was around 36 months. His study, however, did not distinguish word position for LECs. Prather, Hedrick, and Kern (1975) studied the acquisition of the consonantal sounds at both word initial and word final position and Sander's LECs at word initial position were still found to be mastered by children at around 36 months. At word final position, a slightly different pattern appears. On the other hand, Wellman et al. (1931) reported that normally developing children could not produce CSs correctly before 60 months except the cluster /kw/. Smit (1993) also noted that adults learning a second language also found CSs difficult if their first language did not allow the strings. Thus, it is reasonable to assume that CSs are more taxing for the articulators than LECs for both fluent and non-fluent speakers.
In the current study on speakers who stutter, the results for the youngest group of speakers suggest that, before age 12, children who stutter found words starting with CS or LEC difficult. These two factors seem to operate independently in this age group. The focally difficult class of words (CSi-LECi) did not come out as difficult for these speakers in Analysis 3. An inspection of Figure 9 showed that there is an increasing trend for a higher proportion of this class of words to be stuttered as age increases. Speakers in group 1 found content words with LEC at initial position (CSx-LECi and CSi-LECi, where the latter is also focally difficult) more difficult than other content words although not significantly so.
For speakers in group 2, among words without CS at all (CSx), words whose onset involved LECs (CSx-LECi) were more dysfluent than words without LEC at the onset (CSx-LECx and CSx-LECn). The same result no longer existed for speakers in group 3. An inspection of Figure 9 showed that the stuttering ratio of the CSx-LECi is decreasing gradually with age. A significant difference between the CSx-LECi word group and the CSi-LECi word group emerged for the first time in the adult speakers. As age progressed, the speakers might have a better mastery of words starting with LECs and stuttered less on these words. At the same time, the CSn-LECn words, in which CS and LEC may co-occur at non-word-initial positions, were now more dysfluent than CSx-LECx words.
The more difficult words involving CSs at word initial position, however, remains difficult for people who stutter even when they get older. This shows that there may still be a deficit in the articulators in the older speakers whose stuttering persisted or that the therapies they received were not directed to deal with the articulation of consonant strings. Alternatively, the phonetic planning of such words in preparation for motor execution might have remained difficult for adult speakers who stutter.
It was not possible to fully test for ‘focally’ in this study. The main drawback is that only the CSi-LECi class has words with the CSi element. There were not enough occurrences of words containing CSi but not LECi (CSi-LECn and CSi-LECx) in the speech data because most CSi involve LEC in English.
The results of this study show that the two phonological factors, LEC and CS, have an impact on the likelihood that a word would be stuttered or not. The effect depends on the age of the speakers who stutter. It has also been shown that such influence would only apply to content words and at word initial positions but not to function words at any word position. The theory developed by Au-Yeung, Howell, & Pilgrim (1998) and Howell, Au-Yeung, & Sackin (1999) can be used to explain our current findings. The theory explains why the linguistic factors that make a word prone to being stuttered change across ages. The theory, as originally formulated, attempted to account for why stuttering in children occurs predominantly on function words whereas stuttering in older speakers occurs mainly on content words. Stuttering at an early age has similarities with the dysfluencies fluent speakers of all ages produce: Maclay and Osgood's (1959) data on fluent adults show that function words are repeated frequently. Clark and Clark (1977) accounted for this type of dysfluency by proposing that the subsequent word is not prepared for articulation and so the speaker hesitates or repeats the function word or words that have been prepared until the phonetic plan for the word that is not ready has been completed. Au-Yeung, Howell, & Pilgrim (1998) pointed out that the words for which the phonetic plan is not available will tend to be a phonologically more complex content word. Thus, in an example like “and, and, Christine,” the plan for the content word “Christine” is not available and so the speaker delays by repeating the function word “and.” Though this has been described as a mechanism that fluent speakers adopt, it is consistent with the observation that children who stutter exhibit a high proportion of stuttering on clauses starting with the function word “and” (Howell & Au-Yeung, 1995b). Howell, Au-Yeung, & Sackin (1999) provide evidence that they interpret as showing the increased incidence of stuttering on content words in adults is due to these speakers overriding the delaying on the function word. The speakers then have to attempt the content word that is not fully prepared for articulation. This results in the speakers experiencing difficulty in producing content words. Fluent speakers continue to repeat function words when the plan is not available and, consequently, do not encounter this problem. They also propose that the difficulties experienced on content words by people who stutter becomes an automatized response and this makes the problem resistant to removal without treatment (Shiffrin & Schneider, 1977).
Use of the delaying mechanism on function words in children who stutter has two effects: a) The repeated function words are less likely to contain CS and LEC (as shown in Analysis 2); b) When they delay on the function word, they are able to avoid the content words that are more likely to contain phonologically more difficult components such as CS and LEC. Since adults who stutter, on the other hand, attempt content words that are not fully prepared for articulation, the automatization of stuttering on such words is likely to increase the influence of phonological factors like CS and LEC. This theory predicts that as speakers drop repetition of function words, they are obliged to attempt partly prepared content words. This could lead to an increased frequency of stuttering in two ways: First, focally difficult content words may be the ones that are more inclined to have errors in their plan (Levelt, 1989). Second, attempting to execute partly prepared sounds leads to dysfluencies. At present, there are no data available that distinguishes between these alternatives.
In other future work, it will be necessary to look at the exact composition of the CS and its relationship to stuttering. To explain, inspection of the data shows that syllable-initial and syllable-final CSs are usually different from one another. For example, the consonant strings ‘nt’, ‘nd’, ‘lt’, ‘ŋz’, etc. found at syllable-final position are not allowed at syllable-initial position. The same is true for other consonant strings that can only be syllable-initial in English, for example, ‘fr’, ‘str’, ‘kw’, etc. Other strings occur at both initial and final position of a syllable, for example, ‘st’, ‘sk’, etc. This asymmetry alone could account for the word-initial effect of CS. Thus, it will be necessary to establish whether word-initial position of the CS per se, or the inherent difficulty of the CSs that occur in initial position induce stuttering by looking at individual combinations of CSs. Another relevant observation that emphasizes the need for looking at individual CSs is that the phonotactic rules of English lead to most syllable-initial CSs including LECs, whereas LECs do not occur so frequently for syllable-final CSs. In order to carry out such a study, a lot of data is needed so that there are adequate occurrences of each CS type for each speaker.
In the present article, linguistic characterizations of phonological difficulty have been used. It is entirely possible that these abstract characteristics may be related to motor indices. There is evidence in the literatures that some particular sounds or sequences of sounds are more difficult for people who stutter than those who do not (Van Lieshout, et al., 1997). Further research should be conducted to test if words with CS and LEC factors occurring focally at word-initial position are particular motorically taxing for people who stutter.
Finally, a word of caution on our findings is needed. All the results presented are based on speech data from English speakers. Different languages would have a different set of phonological properties. For speakers of other languages, there would be a completely different set of LECs and some languages may not have CS at all in any word positions. These are languages, for example, Walmatjari (an Aboriginal language in Australia; Hudson, 1978), which only allow CV and CVC syllables. Cross-linguistic research is needed to study the common underlying cause of stuttering.
Acknowledgments
This research was supported by a grant from the Wellcome Trust.
REFERENCES
  • Au-Yeung, J; Howell, P. Lexical and syntactic context and stuttering. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics. 1998;12:67–78.
  • Au-Yeung, J; Howell, P; Pilgrim, L. Phonological words and stuttering on function words. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 1998;41:1019–1030.
  • Brown, SF. The loci of stutterings in the speech sequence. Journal of Speech Disorders. 1945;10:182–192.
  • Clark, HH; Clark, E. Psychology and language. An introduction to psycholinguistics. New York: Harcourt; 1977.
  • Dobrich, W; Scarborough, HS. Phonological characteristics of words young children try to say. Journal of Child Language. 1992;19:597–616. [PubMed]
  • Fleiss, JL. Measuring nominal scale agreement among many raters. Psychological Bulletin. 1971;76:378–382.
  • Howell, P; Au-Yeung, J. The association between stuttering, Brown's factors and phonological categories in child stutterers ranging in age between 2 and 12 years. Journal of Fluency Disorders. 1995a;20:331–344.
  • Howell, P; Au-Yeung, J. Syntactic determinants of stuttering in the spontaneous speech of normally fluent and stuttering children. Journal of Fluency Disorders. 1995b;20:317–330.
  • Howell, P; Au-Yeung, J; Sackin, S. Exchange of stuttering from function words to content words with age. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 1999;42:345–354.
  • Hudson, J. The core of Walmatjari grammar. Canberra: Australian Institute of Aboriginal Studies; 1978.
  • Kadi-Hanifi, K; Howell, P. Syntactic analysis of the spontaneous speech of normally fluent and stuttering children. Journal of Fluency Disorders. 1992;17:151–170.
  • Levelt, WJM. Speaking: From intention to articulation. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 1989.
  • Locke, JL. Phonological acquisition and change. NY: Academic Press; 1983.
  • Locke, JL. The sound shape of early lexical representations. In: Smith MD, Locke JL. , editors. The emergent lexicon: The child's development of a linguistic vocabulary. NY: Academic Press; 1988.
  • Maclay, H; Osgood, CE. Hesitation phenomena in spontaneous English speech. Word. 1959;15:169–182.
  • Prather, EM; Hedrick, DL; Kern, CA. Articulation development in children aged two to four years. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders. 1975;40:179–191. [PubMed]
  • Sander, EK. When are speech sounds learned? Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders. 1972;37:55–63. [PubMed]
  • Schwartz, RG. Phonological factors in early lexical acquisition. In: Smith MD, Locke JL. , editors. The emergent lexicon: The child's development of the linguistic vocabulary. NY: Academic Press; 1988.
  • Shiffrin, RM; Schneider, W. Controlled and automatic human information processing: II. Perceptual learning, automatic attending and general theory. Psychological Review. 1977;84:127–190.
  • Slobin, DI. Language change in children and in history. In: Macnamara J. , editor. Language learning and thought. NY: Academic Press; 1977.
  • Smit, AB. Phonologic error distributions in the Iowa-Nebraska Articulation Norms Project: Word-initial consonant clusters. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research. 1993;36:931–947. [PubMed]
  • Smith, BL. The emergent lexicon from a phonetic perspective. In: Smith MD, Locke JL. , editors. The emergent lexicon: The child's development of a linguistic vocabulary. NY: Academic Press; 1988.
  • Throneburg, NR; Yairi, E; Paden, EP. The relation between phonological difficulty and the occurrence of disfluencies in the early stage of stuttering. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research. 1994;37:504–509. [PubMed]
  • Van Lieshout, PHHM; Hulstijn, W; Alfonso, PJ; Peters, HFM. Higher and lower order influences on the stability of the dynamic coupling between articulators. In: Hulstijn W, Peters HFM, Van Lieshout PHHM. , editors. Speech production: Motor control, brain research and fluency disorders. NY: Elsevier Science; 1997.
  • Van Lieshout, PHHM; Starkweather, CW; Hulstijn, W; Peters, H. Effects of linguistic correlates of stuttering on EMG activity in nonstuttering speakers. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research. 1995;38:360–372. [PubMed]
  • Wellman, BL; Case, IM; Mengert, IG; Bradbury, DE. Speech sounds of young children. University of Iowa Studies in Child Welfare. 1931;5
  • Wingate, ME. Early position and stuttering occurrence. Journal of Fluency Disorders. 1982;7:243–258.
  • Wingate, ME. The structure of stuttering: A psycholinguistic analysis. Springer-Verlag; 1988.