Behavioral Research

Table of Contents
1 Self-Efficacy and Health Behavior Theories
2

Measures and Measurements

3

Similar Constructs

4

References

5

Measures Appendix

6 Published Examples

Download Full Text (PDF)

Other Constructs
 

Barriers

 

Dispositional Optimism

 

Environments

 

Illness Representations

  Implementation Intentions
  Intention, Expectation, and Willingness
  Normative Beliefs
  Optimistic Bias
  Perceived Benefits
  Perceived Control
  Perceived Severity
  Perceived Vulnerability
  Self-Efficacy
  Self-Reported Behavior
  Social Influence
  Social Support
  Stages
  Worry

Self-Efficacy
Ralf Schwarzer and Aleksandra Luszczynska

<< Previous

2

Measures and Measurements

General Perceived Self-Efficacy

General self-efficacy assesses a broad and stable sense of personal competence to deal effectively with a variety of stressful situations. This approach is not in opposition to Bandura's (1997) suggestion that self-efficacy should be conceptualized in a situation-specific manner. Rather, general self-efficacy can be used to explain a complex set of adherence behaviors (e.g., in diabetes) or the perception of health or various symptoms. The General Self-Efficacy (GSE) scale (Schwarzer & Jerusalem, 1995) was created to predict coping with daily hassles as well as adaptation after experiencing various kinds of stressful life events (see Appendix). Preferably, the 10 items are randomly mixed into a larger pool of items that have the same response format. It requires 4 minutes on average to answer the questions. Responses are made on a 4-point scale. Responses to all 10 items are summed up to yield the final composite score, with a range from 10 to 40. No recoding. In samples from 23 nations, Cronbach's alphas ranged from .76 to .90. The scale is unidimensional, as found in a series of confirmatory factor analyses (Scholz, Gutiérrez-Doña, Sud, & Schwarzer, 2002). Criterion-related validity is documented in numerous correlation studies where positive coefficients were found with favorable emotions, dispositional optimism, and work satisfaction. Negative coefficients were found with depression, anxiety, stress, burnout, and health complaints. In studies with cardiac patients, their recovery over a half-year time period could be predicted by presurgery self-efficacy (for an overview of validation studies see Luszczynska, Gutiérrez-Doña, & Schwarzer, 2005).

Perceived Self-Efficacy for Health Behaviors

A number of studies on the adoption of health practices have measured self-efficacy to assess its potential influences in initiating behavior change. Often single-item measures or very brief scales (e.g., 4 items) have been used. It is actually not necessary to use larger scales if a specific behavior is to be predicted. More important is rigorous theory-based item wording. A rule of thumb is to use the following semantic structure: "I am certain that I can do xx, even if yy (barrier)" (Luszczynska & Schwarzer, 2005). If the target behavior is less specific, one can either go for more items that jointly cover the area of interest, or develop a few specific subscales. Whereas general self-efficacy measures refer to the ability to deal with a variety of stressful situations, measures of self-efficacy for health behaviors refer to beliefs about the ability to perform certain health behaviors. These behaviors may be defined broadly (i.e., healthy food consumption) or in a narrow way (i.e., consumption of high-fiber food).

Nutrition-Related or Dietary Self-Efficacy
Dieting, weight control, and preventive nutrition can be governed by nutrition self-efficacy beliefs. It has been found that nutrition self-efficacy operates best in concert with general changes in lifestyle, including physical exercise and provision of social support. Self-confident clients in intervention programs were less likely to relapse to their previous unhealthy diets (Bagozzi & Edwards, 1998; Brug, Hospers, & Kok, 1997; Fuhrmann & Kuhl, 1998; Gollwitzer & Oettingen, 1998).

Nutrition self-efficacy has been shown to be a significant predictor of physical, social and self-evaluative outcome expectancies regarding healthy nutrition (Anderson, Winett, & Wojcik, 2000). A study using an objective measure of nutrition behavior, namely grocery receipts, demonstrated that the effect of dietary fiber self-efficacy on fat, fiber, fruit and vegetable intake was mediated by physical outcome expectations. Nutrition goal setting was linked to higher dietary fiber self-efficacy and actual fiber intake (see Schnoll & Zimmerman, 2001). In a similar study, self-efficacy to eat more fruit and vegetables as well as outcome expectancies in terms of fruit and vegetable intake predicted a 24-hour recall of actual fruit and vegetable intake (Resnicow et al., 2000). Additionally, these fruit- and vegetable-specific predictors were inversely related to an unhealthy diet, that is, high-fat cooking.

Studies aimed at predicting nutrition in vulnerable populations or patients with chronic or terminal diseases usually provide support for SCT. The nutrition of women 65 years or older has been found to be related to current nutrition self-efficacy, but not to outcome expectancies (Conn, 1997). Besides knowledge about proper nutrition, dietary self-efficacy and perceived spousal support were associated with dietary behaviors amongst Type 2 diabetes patients (Savoca & Miller, 2001). Diabetes-related self-efficacy was found to be strongly related to maintenance of diabetes self-care (diet, exercise and glucose testing; see Bond, 2002). The most powerful effects were observed when strong optimistic self-beliefs were combined with strong beliefs about outcomes (Bond, 2002). Nutrition and exercise self-efficacy were also connected to the maintenance of diet and physical activity in breast cancer patients (Pinto et al., 2002).

The measurement of this kind of self-efficacy aims at statements that include control over the temptation to eat too much or to choose the wrong foods. Items can include particular foods, such as "I am certain that I can eat five portions of fruits and vegetables per day," or can refer to self-regulatory efforts, as in the example in the Appendix. Another way to assess nutrition self-efficacy was presented by Anderson et al. (2000). Self-efficacy was a predictor of nutrition behavior among shoppers (cf. scale in the Appendix). Some instruments target very specific components of nutrition, such as fat intake in specific populations. An example of scale development is the following: Chang, Nitzke, and Brown (2003) have developed a self-efficacy measure for eating low-fat diets in low-income women.

Physical Exercise Self-Efficacy

Perceived exercise self-efficacy has been found to be a major instigating force in forming intentions to exercise and in maintaining the practice for an extended time (Dzewaltowski, Noble, & Shaw, 1990; Feltz & Riessinger, 1990; McAuley, 1992 , 1993; Shaw, Dzewaltowski, & McElroy, 1992; Weinberg, Grove, & Jackson, 1992; Weiss, Wiese, & Klint, 1989). The measurement of exercise self-efficacy may relate to a specific task, such as "I am certain that I can run for half an hour without stopping, even uphill." Or the target behavior is not directly specified in favor of explicit barriers, as shown in the example (cf. Appendix). In the context of patient education (Lorig et al., 1996), physical activity self-efficacy scales have been used (cf. Appendix). The role of efficacy beliefs in initiating and maintaining a regular program of physical exercise has also been studied by Desharnais, Bouillon, and Godin (1986), Long and Haney (1988), Sallis et al. (1986), Sallis, Hovell, Hofstetter, and Barrington (1992), and Wurtele and Maddux (1987). Endurance in physical performance was found to depend on exercise efficacy beliefs that were created in a series of experiments on competitive efficacy by Weinberg, Gould, and Jackson (1979), Weinberg, Gould, Yukelson, and Jackson (1981), and Weinberg, Yukelson, and Jackson (1980). In terms of competitive performance, tests of the role of efficacy beliefs in tennis performance revealed that perceived exercise efficacy was related to 12 rated performance criteria (Barling & Abel, 1983). A study on vigorous physical activity among school girls provided strong support for SCT, modest support for the TPB, and only weak support for the TRA (Motl et al., 2002). Exercise self-efficacy was the strongest predictor of moderate and vigorous physical activity, whereas behavioral control predicted only vigorous activity.

Alcohol Consumption and Self-Efficacy

This area of research can be subdivided into controlled drinking self-efficacy, drinking refusal self-efficacy, and abstinence self-efficacy. It is relevant in clinical research on alcohol dependence, binge drinking, and relapse prevention, but also primary prevention to examine how youngsters believe that they can resist the temptation to drink. Heavy drinkers had lower abstinence self-efficacy than those who drink less or who drink only in social situations (Christiansen, Vik, & Jarchow, 2002). For alcohol consumption, instruments were presented by Rychtarik, Prue, Rapp, and King (1992), Sitharthan and Kavanagh (1990), and Young, Oei, and Crook (1991).

Most of the research on self-efficacy and drinking behavior has used the Situational Confidence Questionnaire (SCQ; Annis, 1984, 1987). Ratings of self-efficacy have predicted drinking behavior (Annis & Davis, 1988). Adolescents with substance abuse, anxiety, and conduct disorder, diagnostic combinations were found to have significantly lower self-efficacy scores across all the SCQ subscales. Sitharthan, Soames Job, Kavanagh, Sitharthan, and Hough (2003) reported the factor structure of a 20-item Controlled Drinking Self-Efficacy Scale (CDSES) that included four factors: negative affect, positive mood/social context, frequency of drinking, and consumption quantity. The CDSES can be a useful measure in treatment programs providing a moderation drinking goal. Assessing one's self-efficacy to reduce alcohol consumption follows the same pattern as in the previous examples. Items can target a highly specific behavior such as "I am certain that I can refuse a drink tonight when my buddies offer it to me." Or it can be less specific, as proposed by Schwarzer and Renner (2000; see Appendix). There are specific self-efficacy measures that have been developed primarily for problem drinkers who seek a moderation drinking goal.

The Drinking Refusal Self-Efficacy Questionnaire (DRSEQ) by Young and Oei (1996) assesses the belief in one's ability to resist alcohol. A revised factor structure (DRSEQ-R) was confirmed in community, student, and clinical samples. The DRSEQ-R was also found to have good construct and concurrent validity. DiClemente, Carbonari, and Montgomery (1994) have developed the 20-item Alcohol Abstinence Self-Efficacy scale (AASE). There were 174 men and 92 women who came to an outpatient alcoholism treatment clinic. Ratings were made on a 5-point Likert scale of confidence to abstain from alcohol across 20 different high-risk situations. A parallel set of items assessed the participants' temptation to drink in each situation. The four 5-item subscales of the AASE measured types of relapse precipitants labeled negative affect, social positive, physical and other concerns, and withdrawal and urges.

Smoking Cessation Self-Efficacy

Confidence to overcome barriers (i.e., smoking cessation self-efficacy) can predict attempts to quit smoking (Dijkstra & DeVries, 2000). Nicotine abstinence of self-quitters depends on various demographic, physiological, cognitive and social factors, but only a few factors are common predictors of maintaining abstinence. These are physiological factors, such as lower nicotine dependence, longer duration of previous abstinence, and, as a cognitive factor, high perceived smoking cessation self-efficacy (see Ockene et al., 2000).

Poor smoking cessation self-efficacy is associated with lapses. Coping successfully with high-risk situations as they occur during the maintenance period is dependent on self-efficacy. Confidence in one's ability to abstain from smoking might refer to particular environmental or affective contexts, such as feelings of irritation or sadness, socializing with smokers, or being in a bar or a restaurant. Gwaltney et al. (2002) found that lapse episodes within a four-week abstinence period were predicted by abstinence self-efficacy. Abstinence self-efficacy differentiated between the temptation episodes in which the former smoker was able to resist smoking and situations that ended up with lapses. In a study on lapses and relapses of smokers who attempted to quit, self-efficacy was measured daily in order to analyze whether changes in optimistic self beliefs precede lapses during 25 days after quitting smoking (Shiffman et al., 2000). On days when both groups were abstinent, persons who never lapsed during the monitoring period reported higher daily self-efficacy than those who lapsed. Daily average self-efficacy over the lapse-to-relapse interval was lower among persons who relapsed than daily average postlapse self-efficacy among those who did not. Self-efficacy after the lapse significantly predicted subsequent behavior.

The classic smoking self-efficacy measurement is the scale by Colletti, Supnick, and Payne (1985). They developed the 17-item SSEQ, an instrument designed to measure self-efficacy for resisting the urge to smoke. Participants were treated in a behaviorally oriented smoking reduction program. Analyses based upon SSEQ ratings, carbon monoxide measurements, self-reported smoking rate, and relapse data supported the predictions derived from self-efficacy theory. Smoking abstinence self-efficacy ratings should be context-specific; they should vary across situations. This variability could signal high risk for relapse situations. In a study by Gwaltney, Shiffman, and Normal (2001), the Relapse Situation Efficacy Questionnaire (RSEQ) was developed. Results showed that both context-specific and unidimensional measures of self-efficacy were relevant. Context-specific factors included Negative Affect, Positive Affect, Restrictive Situations (to smoking), Idle Time, Social-Food Situations, Low Arousal, and Craving. These factors predicted cessation outcome, after controlling for concurrent smoking rate.

Some smoking cessation self-efficacy scales were composed in a multidimensional manner. In one study (Dijkstra & De Vries, 2000), the following five subscales were constructed: Emotional self-efficacy, Social self-efficacy, Skill self-efficacy, Relapse self-efficacy and Try self-efficacy. Twenty-seven items were used in the questionnaire: Situational Self-Efficacy, 8 items; Skill Self-Efficacy, 9 items; Relapse Self-Efficacy, 5 items; Try Self-Efficacy, 5 items (see Appendix).

In a sample of smokers with low motivation to quit, two subsequent self-report measurements of smoking cessation self-efficacy were conducted. It was found that quitting history and smoking behavior were related to the types of self-efficacy. Number of past attempts at quitting was only related to relapse self-efficacy. Only skill self-efficacy was predictive of quitting activity between Time 1 and Time 2. Point prevalence quitting at Time 2 was predicted by Skill self-efficacy and relapse self-efficacy. The more clearly the means to accomplish the task are specified, the more valid the self-efficacy judgments were (Dijkstra, Bakker, & DeVries, 1997).

Adherence to Medication Self-Efficacy and Rehabilitation Self-Efficacy

Poor compliance with recommended treatment may result partly from patients' experience of adverse side effects, but it may also be due to a lack of self-regulatory skills. Considering psychosocial factors, adherence is related to lack of social support and lack of self-efficacy beliefs about one's ability to adhere to medication (Catz, Kelly, Bogart, Benotsch, & McAuliffe, 2000). For example, Molassiotis et al. (2002) have found that adherence to antiretroviral medication in patients with HIV was strongly related to self-efficacy (that is, optimistic self-beliefs about the ability to follow the medication regimen). These self-beliefs, together with anxiety and nausea, were related to adherence to the recommended treatment. The relation between social support and medication adherence was weaker than the relation between self-efficacy and medication adherence. Low adherence self-efficacy, together with low outcome expectancies regarding the benefits following the treatment regimen, have also been found to be related to low medication adherence in HIV symptomatic women or women with AIDS (Murphy, Greenwell, & Hoffman, 2002).

Some medications must be self- injected on a frequent basis. A study by Mohr, Boudewyn, Likosky, Levine, and Goodkin (2001) examined cognitive and affective contributions to the ability to self-inject and adherence among patients with a relapsing form of MS. Pretreatment injection self-efficacy expectations were related to 6-month adherence.

A study by Kobau and Dilorio (2003) described self-efficacy beliefs and outcome expectancies toward medication, seizure, and lifestyle management behaviors among adults with epilepsy. Participants responded to the Epilepsy Self-Efficacy scale addressing the confidence for following medication dosing schedule, planning for medication refills, coping with adverse effects of medication, getting sufficient sleep, avoiding alcohol, and obtaining social support. Those with low self-efficacy are expected to benefit from self-efficacy interventions to enhance their self-management ability.

Diabetes self-efficacy was associated with a measure of metabolic control in insulin-dependent diabetes patients (Grossmann, Brink, & Hauser, 1987). Authors used a 35-item measure, the Self-Efficacy for Diabetes scale. Adolescents rated on a 5-point scale how much they were confident that they could implement tasks specific for four components of insulin-dependent diabetes management (insulin injections, blood glucose monitoring, dietary prescriptions, exercise).

A medication adherence self-efficacy scale was developed by Gbenga, Mancuso, Allegrante, and Charlson (2003) in ambulatory hypertensive African-American patients in two sequential phases. After an initial test with a 43-item self-efficacy questionnaire, 26 items were retained for the final self-efficacy scale. This scale can be used to identify risk situations in adherence to prescribed medications.

Condom Use Self-Efficacy

Condom use self-efficacy has been studied to explain unprotected sexual behavior, such as not using contraceptives to avoid unwanted pregnancies. Teenage girls with a high rate of intercourse have been found to use contraceptives more effectively if they believe they could exercise control over their sexual activities (Levinson, 1982; Wang, Wang, & Hsu, 2003). Most of the studies referring to risky sexual behaviors have examined social cognitive predictors of condom use. Optimistic beliefs in one's capability to negotiate safer sex practices emerged as the most important predictor of protective behaviors (Basen-Engquist, 1992; Kasen, Vaughn, & Walter, 1992; Wulfert & Wan, 1993). Brafford and Beck (1991) have developed the Condom Use Self-Efficacy scale, consisting of 28 items describing an individual's feelings of confidence about being able to purchase and use condoms. Among college students, those who differed on levels of previous condom use as well as on sexual intercourse experience also showed differences on this scale in the expected direction. Giles, Liddell, and Bydawell (2005) have studied self-efficacy in the context of the TPB to predict and explain condom use in young adults at two points in time. Condom use self-efficacy was designed to measure the extent to which an individual believes he/she has the confidence/ability to use a condom, such as "I believe I have the ability to use a condom the next time I have sex." Results provided some support for TPB. Subjective norm and self-efficacy emerged as substantial predictors. Kaljee, Genberg, and Riel (2005) present findings from a randomized-controlled trial of an HIV prevention program for adolescents. They used the Vietnamese Youth Health Risk Behavior Instrument, including scales based on the protection motivation theory (PMT). The modified instrument included an 8-item self-efficacy scale with items such as:

  1. I could get condoms if I wanted to.
  2. I could put a condom on correctly.
  3. I could convince the person I am having sex with that we should use a condom, even if he doesn't want to.
  4. I could ask for condoms in a pharmacy.

The data suggest the potential applicability of the PMT for HIV program development with non-Western adolescents.

Detective Behaviors and Self-Efficacy

Some studies provided evidence that both outcome expectancies and perceived self-efficacy were the best joint predictors of the intention to engage in regular breast cancer detection behaviors (Meyerowitz & Chaiken 1987; Seydel, Taal, & Wiegman, 1990).

Some screening behaviors refer to actions that are performed by individuals without any contact with health practitioners. These include breast or testicular self-examination. An example of a breast self-examination (BSE) self-efficacy scale was developed by Luszczynska and Schwarzer (2003). Additionally, the authors divided BSE self-efficacy into those referring to forming an intention (preaction BSE self-efficacy), and those referring to the maintenance of BSE (maintenance BSE self-efficacy). These types of BSE self-efficacy were related to the intention to perform BSE and to behavior, respectively. The scale has good reliability (alphas of .81 and .77) and a two-factorial structure (cf. Luszczynska & Schwarzer, 2003). The scale is presented in the Appendix.

A study on first-degree relatives of prostate cancer patients supports the role of self-efficacy for screening behaviors. Physician recommendation, knowledge, and risk estimation were only poor predictors, whereas self-efficacy beliefs and positive outcome expectancies were more closely linked to prostate cancer screening ( Cormier, Kwan, Reid, & Litwin, 2002). Participation in endoscopic colorectal cancer screening was predicted by self-efficacy, followed by the individuals' beliefs about the outcome of participation. Self-efficacy discriminated between those who participated in the screening and those who did not ( Kremers, Mesters, Pladdet, van den Borne, & Stockbrügger, 2000).

Cancer screening might be a first step on the way to change everyday health behaviors. However, people who enrolled in a single cancer screening (e.g., lung cancer) do not necessarily have high self-efficacy regarding behaviors related to this cancer (e.g., regular screening, quitting smoking). Among those women who smoked heavily who were asked to participate in lung cancer screening, almost two thirds were classified as having low self-efficacy regarding smoking cessation (Schnoll et al., 2002), while most of them (76 per cent) reported high positive outcome expectancies of quitting. The results are in line with SCT, which emphasizes the role of self-efficacy in the process of behavior change. Despite relatively high expectations about quitting smoking, the participants were unable to change their smoking habits.

<< Previous

Search | Help | Contact Us | Accessibility | Privacy Policy

DCCPSNational Cancer Institute Department of Health and Human Services National Institutes of Health USA.gov

Health Behavior Constructs: Theory, Measurement, & Research  You can Quit smoking Now! smokefree.gov