Survival of Honey Bees, Apis mellifera (Hymenoptera: Apidae), Fed Various Pollen Sources

JUSTIN 0. SCHMIDT, STEVEN C. THOENES, AND M. D. LEVIN


Agricultural Research Service, U.S. Department of Agriculture, Carl Hayden Bee Research Center, 2000 East Allen Road, Tucson, Arizona 85719





Ann. Entomol. Soc. Am. 80: 176-183 (1987)

ABSTRACT Twenty-five pure pollens plus several blends of pollen were fed as sole protein sources to honey bees, Apis mellifera L., and resultant survival of bees on these diets was measured. Average increase in mean life span of bees on a pollen diet versus sugar water controls was 19.5 d, with a range from 3.9 d less than the controls for Ambrosia to 40.6 d longer, for a five-pollen blend. Actual consumption of test pollen diets also varied dramatically among test pollens, with a mean consumption of 16.5 mg pollen per bee for the first 10 d and a range of 1.9-29.0 mg per bee. Pollens that induced decreased life span included Ambrosia, Uromyces (a rust spore), Typha, and Kallstroemia; those that induced little increase in life span included Haplopappus, Baccharis, and Taraxacum; and those that induced the greatest increase in life span included Ephedra, Prosopis, Rubus, Populus, and two blends of pollen. Increased life span was not correlated with pollen grain size, grain spininess, or dispersal vector (biotic versus abiotic) and was weakly correlated with season of floral bloom (pollen produced in spring was better than that produced in fall). Pollen from the family Compositae was poorer than average pollen. Major factors affecting life span were amount of pollen consumed, protein concentration in the pollen, and, especially, total amount of pollen protein consumed. By combining information relating to consumption levels and protein intake, reasons why a pollen caused the observed life span could be determined. Reasons included presence or absence of attractants and phagostimulaDts/deterrents, presence of toxic compounds, and a poor nutrient balance or level.

KEY WORDS Apis mellifera, pollen, survival




ONE OF THE most enduring apicultural research problems has been the development and evaluation of artificial diets for honey bees, Apis mellifera L., (Haydak 1945, Moeller 1967, Standifer et al. 1973, Doull et al. 1980, Herbert & Shimanuki 1980, Winston et al. 1983). Natural or artificial diets can be evaluated by a variety of means, the relationships between which are often obscure. To be of nutritional value a diet must be collected, have a proper texture and consistency, and then be consumed. Once ingested, the diet must not be toxic, provide most or all of the nutritive elements essential to growth and development, be digestible, and be absorbed through the gut and into the hemolymph. If any of these steps does not occur, the diet will not provide proper nutrition for bees. Because of the complexity of factors involved in honey bee nutrition, we have chosen to investigate the nutritional characteristics of pollen, the natural food of honey bees, rather than artificial diets.

In the past, our work has focused on factors affecting the collection (Schmidt 1982), consumption (Schmidt 1984, 1985, Schmidt & Johnson 1984), and digestion and utilization of pollen (Schmidt & Buchmarm 1985). By looking in this way at various individual pieces of the honey bee dietary Rosetta stone, we hope to decipher the code. This may allow development of effective meridic protein diets as replacements for pollen.

We report here the results of a technique used to evaluate the effect of diets consisting of different pollen sources on the life span of newly emerged honey bees.

Materials and Methods



 

Pollen. The pollens tested are listed in Table 1. Except for Typha latifolia L., which was handcollected from male inflorescences, all the pollen consisted of corbicular pellets obtained from pollen traps maintained on honey bee colonies. All collections occurred during the bloom periods listed in Table I and most were derived from the Tucson, Ariz., area. Exceptions included Cynara obtained from California south of San Francisco; Prunus dulcis Batsch from Winters, Calif.; Rubus from Kirkland, Wash.; and Taraxacurn from Laramie, Wyo.

Pollen for the tests was obtained by sorting beecollected pellets. A pollen was used only when it could be separated on the basis of color from a particular blend of known species. Any pellets (<3%) that contained a blend from two floral sources were discarded. All test pollens and the other pollens in the blends from which the test pollen was removed were determined by microscopic examination. After collection, all pollen was stored at -20*C until used in the tests. Several pollen mixtures were tested in addition to the pure pollens listed in Table 1. These included a standard identified mixture of 15 spring-collected species (Schmidt & Johnson 1984); a 2:2:1 (by weight) mixture of Cereus, Prosopis, and Larrea; and an equal mixture of Cereus, Prosopis, Larrea, Prunus, and Populus. These mixtures were selected because the standard mixture served as a reference for all of our experiments, the three- pollen mixture represented the three major pollen sources in the Tucson area, and the five-pollen mixture consisted of a blend of pollen with good as well as poor texture that was preferred as well as less preferred by the bees (Schmidt 1984). Properties of pollen, including grain size and protein content, were determined as in Schmidt & Johnson (1984).

Bees and Survival Tests. Honey bees used in the tests were obtained from colonies of mostly Italian honey bees located in Tucson, Ariz. Several frames of pupal brood were taken randomly from one to three colonies and maintained until emergence in boxes kept in an environmental room at 32-35'C in constant darkness and 70% RH.

Tests were conducted using 60 bees 1 d old removed from the emergence boxes and placed in acrylic plastic and screen cages (9 by 6 by 15 cm) in the environmental room. Each cage was provided water, a piece of beeswax foundation for the bees to rest upon, 50% sucrose solution, and a test pollen. The test pollen was formed by mixing corbicular pellets with distilled water sufficient to achieve a moist, kneadable texture. For Typha pollen, which was the only hand-collected pollen or spore, 30% sucrose by weight was added before mixing with water to provide a sugar level approximately equal to that normally added by bees in the process of making their corbicular loads (Schmidt & Buchmarm 1986). Each test pollen mixture was then placed in the hollows of polyethylene 24/40 tapered flask stoppers (taper from 24 to 21 mm diameter with a hollow 19 turn diameter by 33 turn long) (Nalgene Plastics), and the stoppers were inserted into holes slanted at 200 in the sides of the cages. Controls were identical to treatments except they received only sucrose solution and no pollen.

Three to six different tests plus a control were run concurrently. Each test and control consisted of three replicates. Each Monday, Wednesday, and Friday dead bees in each replicate were counted and removed, and pollen was replaced with fresh pollen. Weight of the consumed pollen was obtained by determining weight loss of the pollen remaining and subtracting from that loss the evaporative water loss of a matched stopper with pollen mix in a beeless cage in the environmental room. During the 2-d periods between changes, the diets did not appear to harden. Pollen-feeding continued for 20 d, by which time all measureable pollen-feeding had ceased.

Analysis. Mortality curves were generated with probit analyses using a SAS Proc Probit version 5.03 (SAS Institute 1982). From this program, 25, 50, and 75% mortality values plus the upper and lower 95% CL were generated. To simplify the data we report only the values plus or minus the interval equal to the greatest difference between either 95% CL and the central value. Kruskal-Wallis H test analyses were performed according to Wolff (1968), and linear regressions were calculated with SYSTAT version 2.0 (SYSTAT, Evanston, Ill.).

Results





Typical mortality curves for bees fed various pollen diets are shown in Fig. 1. Control bees lived ca. 20 d, after which time mortality greatly increased, and all were dead by the 35th d (Fig. 1 A). When fed Kallstroemia, a species generally not collected or readily consumed by honey bees, survival curves were similar to those of controls (Fig. 1B). Larrea, one of the main honey bee nectar and pollen sources in the Sonoran Desert, promoted greater mean survival and increased the maximum life span by ca. 20 d (Fig. 1C). Finally, one of the best pollen sources for bees, the mixture of five different pollens, greatly enhanced survival, with some individuals living to >= d (Fig. 1D). Bees consuming pollen that promoted increased survival also tended to die more uniformly over time; controls had precipitous die-offs (Fig. 1D versus 1A).

Less typical mortality curves are shown in Fig. 2. Bees fed pollen from Washingtonia gradually died off over 75 d (Fig. 2A); those fed Simmondsia did not start dying until the 25th d, then died uniformly over the next 35-40 d (Fig. 2B); bees fed Cynara exhibited irregular periods of high dieoff (Fig. 2C); and those replicates fed Leucophyllum showed inconsistent patterns (Fig. 2D).

Each test series involved bees of similar genetics, nutritional background, and environmental situation. All of these conditions were different among bees of different test series. Thus, to avoid major influence of these variables, the best measure for determining the value of a diet is comparison of survival of the bees on that diet with survival of the bees in the matched control. When this is done, a value of days of additional survival time over that of the control is obtained. This added survival time value also allows more valid comparisons among all the different pollens.



The results of the mean increase in survival are given in Table 2. In this table, the best-performing diet measured at the 50% mortality time was the five-pollen blend. The bees on this diet survived 41 d longer than the controls. The poorest diet, Ambrosia, actually caused a decrease of 4 d in the average life span of the bees compared with the controls. The 25 and 75% mortality values followed trends similar to the 50% mortality curve.

In general, the pollen mixtures increased survival time relative to pure pollen sources. The mean increase in survival for an average bee fed one of the four blends of pollen (two tests of the standard mixture and one each of the three-pollen and fivepollen mixtures) was 29 d. In comparison, an average increase of only 19.5 d was observed for all of the diets tested and 22.7 d for the separate pollen components (Larrea, Prunus, Prosopis, Cereus, Populus). Several of the diets were tested twice, often with significantly different lengths of survival. These differences are attributable to differing conditions before and during the trials for the two analyses, differences that are not easy to quantify (e.g., effect of season and juvenile hormone levels on worker life spans [Fluri et al. 19771).

The average total life span of the bees fed various diets ranged from 22 to 62 d, with a mean of 42 d. The time for 25% of the bees on a diet to die ranged from 17 to 42 d, with an average of 30 d; for 75% of the bees to die the time ranged from 25 to 84 d, with 54 d as the average. The average life span of the controls for the seven different experimental diet series ranged from 15 to 34 d, with a mean of 24 d; the time for 25% of the controls to die ranged from 9 to 29 d, with a mean of 19 d; the time for 75% to die ranged from 19 to 40 d, with a mean of 29 d. The values for added days of survival for bees at the 25, 50, or 75% mortality levels in Table 2 can be converted into values for total life spans from eclosion by adding 19, 24, and 29 d to the respective table values.

The bees generally consumed pollen at the greatest rate during the first 10 d. Thereafter, consumption decreased sharply and ceased by the 20th d. The rate of pollen consumption for six of the pollens is shown in Fig. 3. Two of the most preferred pollens, the standard mix (Fig. 3A) and Populus (Fig. 3B), illustrate typical rapid food intake; by ca. the 8th-10th d >75% of the total pollen consumption had occurred. A similar situation was observed for Baccharis, a less-preferred type (Fig. 3C); the main difference was that amounts consumed were lower and termination of feeding sharper at the 8th d. Ambrosia (Fig. 3D) and Typha (Fig. 3E) both were consumed in large-to-moderate amounts; nevertheless, both caused a net decrease in survival time relative to controls (Table 2). In the case of Ambrosia, pollen consumption ceased abruptly at the 5th d. A last example, Kallstroemia (Fig. 3F), illustrates a pollen that essentially is rejected by the bees. That they also died earlier than the controls suggests that Kallstroemia is at least deterrent to bees and probably toxic as well.

Table 3 lists the amount of pollen consumed per bee during the initial 10-d feeding period and for the entire 20 d of feeding. Also listed is the protein content in percentage of dry weight of each species and the total amount of pollen protein consumed per bee during the main 10-d feeding period. Pollen consumption ranged from 1.9 to 29 mg per bee, with a mean of 16.5 mg. At one extreme, Kallstroemia was essentially refused by the bees, whereas the five-pollen blend was avidly consumed. In terms of total pollen protein consumed, the values ranged from 0.3 to 7.1 mg, with a mean. value of 3.2 mg. The extreme values were again represented by Kallstroemia and the five-pollen blend.

The average life span of bees on test pollen increased significantly with increasing protein concentration in the pollen (r2 = o.33; df = 31; P = 0.001), with an increase in the 10- and 20-d amounts of pollen consumed r^ 2 = 0.44; df = 31; P < 0.001. r^2 = 0.49; df = 31; P < 0.001), and with an increase in the 10- and 20-d total amounts of protein consumed r^2 2 = 0.65; df = 31; P < 0.001. r^2 = 0.62; df = 31; P < 0.001).

Factors other than pollen consumption and protein levels were evaluated to determine if they affected the survival of bees. Of the 25 species of pollen tested, six were in the Compositae, two each were in the Rosaceae and Zygophyllaceae, and 15 belonged to unique families (Table 1). The only family with enough species to evaluate was the Compositae. The pollen from the six species in the Compositae was significantly poorer X^2 = 5.84; df = 1; P < 0.05; Kruskal-Wallis H test) in increasing life span than average pollen. Springblooming (January-June) species on average produce pollen that increases life span more than fall-blooming (July-December) species (x' = 3.93; df = 1; P < 0.05; Kruskal-Wallis H test). An additional point to note is that the last two pollen flows of the year, Haplopappus and Baccharis, both of which are major pollen flows, produced very poor pollen from the standpoint of protein content, honey bee consumption, and increasing life span of the bees.

Of the 25 tested pollen species, 17 are zoophilous and seven are anemophilous, or wind-pollinated (Table 1). Three of the anemophilous species, Ambrosia, Uromyces, and Typha, were the poorest species overall in increasing honey bee life span (they actually decreased it), whereas Populus, Ephedra, and Simmondsia were among the highest in increasing honey bee life span. Thus, the mechanism of pollen transfer (animal or wind) per se does not correlate with the ability of bees to survive on that pollen.

Finally, it is possible that the size or surface texture of the individual pollen grains play some role in the survival of bees fed different pollens. However, pollen size within the range of 20 to ca. 100 X 10^-6 m (greatest length or diameter) does not appear to play a role. For example, the three best and worst pollens in Table 3 all have average-sized pollen grains (<45 Am diameter), whereas the largest pollen grains, Agave (100 X 10^-6 m), Malva, Cynara, and Kallstroemia (60 X 10^-6 m), were, with the exception of Kallstroernia, mostly average in their ability to increase life span (size data in Schmidt & Johnson [1984]). Most of the pollen species have smooth or reticulate outer surface sculpture, but six (Haplopappus, Baccharis, Taraxacum, spring composites, Cynara, and Malva) have spiny pollen. Spininess, however, does not appear either to reduce consumption of the pollen or to depress bee life span (P > 0.1; Kruskal- Wallis H test).



Discussion


A. mellifera is the most successful of all bee species in ecologically exploiting the various environments of the world. This is partly because, when necessary, honey bees are nearly catholic in their collection and consumption of the varied pollen resources present in their environments. Alhough they are noted for this extreme polylectic behavior, honey bees rarely collect foods other than pollen, and when they do it is usually under extreme conditions of pollen shortage. Honey bees not only are capable of distinguishing between pollen and nonpollen, they also are capable, when the opportunities arise, of discriminating actively among various pollen sources (Levin & Bohart 1955, Doull 1966, Schmidt 1982, Schmidt & Johnson 1984). The basis upon which bees discriminate against nonpollen sources and some pollen (e.g., cotton, Gossypiurn hirsutum L.; onion, Allium cepa L.; some Cucurbitaceae; Kallstroernia) is not known. Likewise, how bees almost always recognize most pollens as food is not known, although there is evidence that attractants and phagostimulants are involved (Doull 1966, Robinson & Nation 1968, Schmidt 1985).

Honey bee feeding behavior consists of a distinct series of behavioral and physiological processes. Nectar, honeydew, soft drinks, and other sugar-rich fluids are best viewed as simple energy sources needed by bees to power their flight, to synthesize wax, and for thermoregulation. As such, these materials are of little nutritional interest and are not discussed further. Pollen, on the other hand, provides all the nutrients needed by bees for growth and development rather than energy production. Pollen-feeding behavior consists first of recognition and collection of pollen by honey bee foragers, individuals who themselves eat none or very little of the pollen they collect. Once collected, the pollen is transferred to cells in the hive where younger bees then determine if the pollen is acceptable for consumption and, if so, consume it. Whatever excess pollen remains is stored in combs.

To be effective, any natural or artificial diet for honey bees must induce all of the phases of honey bee feeding and then be digested. Part of the problem in evaluating effectiveness of honey bee diets is analyzing the successful completion of all the steps and determining which steps, if any, are causing problems. Increased life span of recently eclosed adults fed a given pollen or test diet is one of several ways to evaluate the effectiveness of a diet. Maurizio (1946), using methodology similar to ours, determined that the life span of honey bees fed 5-20% pollen in a honey-based candy varied depending upon the pollen tested. Milne (1980, 1981) also used life span, in this case of starving bees, to determine successfully which queens produced workers with a propensity for hoarding syrup and for producing honey. Our tests differed from those of Maurizio and Milne in that we provided the bees with a food composed only of pollen plus the bee-added sugar or equivalent, and measured how this pollen affected both life span and consumption. We based many of our calculations on the first 10 d of life, because this time period is well established as the time when most of the pollen consumption by caged bees occurs (Haydak 1970). Because significant additive genetic variation underlies the honey bee life span (Milne 1982), the bees used for all tests initiated on a given date were derived from a mixed genetic pool derived from more than one colony.

Beekeepers in the upland area of the Sonoran Desert of Arizona have long felt that various Compositae produce poor- quality pollen. This, in general, was borne out in these tests. We do not know the reasons for the poor quality, but note that our tested composite pollen was usually low in protein, had a large amount of oily external pollenkitt, and often contained bitter-tasting (to humans) compounds.

Pollen is metabolically costly for plants to produce. As a result, pollen-collecting organisms would be expected to exert selection pressure on plants that are wind-pollinated to evolve methods to prevent theft. One such method to prevent or reduce pollen theft is the production of pollen that is unattractive, poisonous to, or not usable by insects such as honey bees. Clearly, such evolutionary events have not occurred in all of the species in these tests; overall, pollen from wind- pollinated species performed as well as that from animalpollinated species. Two factors merit further mention in this regard: honey bees are not native to the study area and, hence, could not have been a selective force in pollen evolution until recently; and some anemophilous species definitely were not readily collected (Uromyces and Typha) and some were possibly toxic (Ambrosia, Uromyces, Typha). Thus, some anernophilous species might have evolved means to prevent pollen robbing and some might not have.

Two factors that affected the ability of pollen to increase life span are the amount of the pollen consumed and the amount of protein in the pollen. Consumption is partially a measure of how attractive and phagostimulating a pollen is. Readily consumed species were obviously at least not deterrent and almost certainly contained effective attractants and phagostimulants. They were not, however, in all cases, digestible or beneficial, as illustrated by Ambrosia and Typha, which were consumed yet reduced life span. High-protein level alone did not guarantee consumption or effective digestion. For example, Larrea has a high protein level, yet relatively little of it was eaten by the bees. The greatest correlation with increased life span was the total amount of protein ingested. This measure is the product of the amount of pollen eaten and the concentration of protein in the pollen.

Several of the pollen sources produced unusual results. Uromyces, the rust spores, were the only nonpollen source the bees collected that we could test. These spores were black and contained the lowest protein levels of any of the materials tested. Also, the bees consumed almost none of the spores and what they did consume might have been poisonous, as evidenced by more rapid mortality. Honey bees also apparently do not collect Uromyces except under periods of extreme pollen dearth (in this case during the dry period just after the summer rains).

Kallstroemia is a locally abundant floral species that produces brick-red pollen, and its flowers are avidly visited by various beetles, scoliid and tiphiid wasps, and solitary bees. The flowers are rarely visited by honey bees and are presumably pollinated by native wasps or bees. Although the protein levels in Kallstroemia are marginal for honey bees, the pollen probably also contains at least repellents and phagodeterrents, because the bees rarely collect or consume it. The pollen is probably toxic to Apis as well.

Ambrosia is collected and readily consumed. Yet this anemophilous pollen, in spite of being consumed in quantity, reduces longevity and is apparently toxic to bees. Thus, unlike for Uromyces and Kallstroemia, toxicity and attractiveness were clearly separate factors.

Typha is one of the most interesting of the tested pollens. This wind-pollinated species contains large amounts of starch (J.O.S., unpublished data) and is rarely, if ever, collected by honey bees in our area. Yet, this species has been used as the pollen source for rearing numerous generations of the polylectic sweat bee, Lasioglossum zephyrum (Smith) (C. D. Michener, personal communication). In the case of honey bees, this pollen was little consumed and reduced survival in the bees. Apparently it contains repellents (or lacks attractants) and is toxic to honey bees, even though it is a good food for Lasioglossum. Perhaps the difference lies in preparation and the consumer: in Lasioglossum the pollen is formulated with nectar into a pollen ball for the larvae, whereas in Apis we fed it to adults. Possibly, either microbial degradation occurs in the pollen ball or the larvae of Lasioglossum can deal more readily with adverse factors in the pollen than adult Apis.

Populus is a pollen source that caused some surprises. Local beekeepers note that their bees frequently collect large amounts of this pollen, yet they do not feel it is particularly good for the bees (it often stays stored in the colony for long periods of time). Also, Populus pollen, which is a very light tan when freshly collected, rapidly turns brown and finally very dark brown when stored at room temperature. The Populus we fed the bees was freshly frozen (hence, light tan) and was avidly consumed in spite of its low protein level. It was apparently highly nutritious, at least as measured by ability to increase life span, because it promoted the longest survival in bees of any of the pure pollens fed. Possible problems with Populus in the natural setting that could not be observed ed in our tests are the formation of toxins or repellents as it ages and darkens, and the possibility that in the hive nurse bees could not digest this low-protein pollen quickly enough to gain real benefit from it.

Finally, the five-pollen blend was consumed the most avidly of any tested pollen. This pollen contained vary high levels of protein and promoted the greatest increase in life span of any of the diets. Apparently, a good blend of pollen like this not only approximates the natural diet of Apis but also is maximally consumed and nutritious for Apis.

Acknowledgment


We thank John Dugger for assisting in the experiments; Roy Thurber and Dennis Briggs for providing the Rubus and Prunus pollen; Robert Schmalzel for identifying much of the pollen used; Gary Richardson for invaluable help in designing the statistical analysis; Scott Sakaluk, Stephen Buchmann, Joseph Rieth, and J. H. Martin for critically reviewing the manuscript; and Lucille Valente and Cristina Bramley for manuscript preparation.

References Cited


Doull, K. M. 1966. The relative attractiveness to pollen collecting honeybees of some different pollens. J. Apic. Res. 5: 9-14.

Doull, K. M., T. W. Hancock & L. N. Standifer. 1980. The physical characteristics of artificial protein diets for honeybees (Apis mellifera) II. Apidologie 11: 209-215

Fluri, P., H. Willie, L. Gerig & M. Luescher. 1977. juvenile hormone, vitellogenin and hemocyte composition in winter worker honeybees (Apis mellifera). Experientia 33: 1240-1241.

Haydak, M. H. 1945. Value of pollen substitutes for brood rearing of honeybees. J. Econ. Entomol. 38: 484-487. 1970. Honey bee nutrition. Annu. Rev. Entomol. 15: 143-156.

Herbert, E. W., Jr., & H. Shimanuki. 1980. An evaluation of seven potential pollen substitutes for honey bees. Am. Bee J. 120: 349-350.

Levin, M. D. & G. E. Bohart. 1955. Selection of pollens by honey bees. Am. Bee J. 95: 392-393, 402.

Maurizio, A. 1946. Beobachtungen ueber die Lebensdauer und den Futterverbrauch gefangen gehaltenen Bienen. Beih. Schweiz. Bienenzig. 2: 1-48.

Milne, C. P., Jr. 1980. Laboratory measurement of honey production in the honeybee. 2. Longevity or length of life of caged workers. J. Apic. Res. 19: 172-175

1981. Laboratory measurement of honey production in the honeybee. 4. Relationship between responses on tests of hoarding, longevity or length of life and pupal weight of the worker. J. Apic. Res. 20: 28-30.

1982. Early death of newly emerged worker honeybees in laboratory test cages. J. Apic. Res. 21: 107-110.

Moeller,F.E. 1967. Honeybee preference for pollen supplements or substitutes and their use in colony management. Am. Bee J. 107: 48-50.

Robinson, F. A. & J. L. Nation. 1968. Substances that attract caged honeybee colonies to consume pollen supplements and substitutes. J. Apic. Res. 7: 83-88

SAS Institute. 1982. SAS user's guide, statistics. SAS Institute, Cary, N.C.

Schmidt, J. 0. 1982. Pollen foraging preferences of honey bees. Southwest. Entomol. 7: 255-259.

1984. Feeding preferences of Apis mellifera L. (Hymenoptera: Apidae): individual versus mixed pollen species. J. Kans. Entomol. Soc. 57: 323-327.

1985. Phagostimulants in pollen. J. Apic. Res. 24: 107-114.

Schmidt, J. 0. & S. L. Buchmann. 1985. Pollen digestion and nitrogen utilization by Apis mellifera L. (Hymenoptera: Apidae). Comp. Biochem. Physiol. A 82: 499-503.

1986. Floral biology of the saguaro (Cereus giganteus). I. Pollen harvest by Apis mellifera. Oecologia (Berlin) 69: 490-498.

Schmidt, J. 0. & B. E. Johnson. 1984. Pollen feeding preference of Apis mellifera, a polylectic bee. Southwest. Entomol. 9: 41-47.

Standifer, L. N., M. H. Haydak, J. P. Mills & M. D. Levin. 1973. Value of three protein rations in maintaining honeybee colonies in outdoor flight cages. J. Apic. Res. 12: 137-143.

Winston, M. L., W. T. Chalmers & P. C. Lee. 1983. Effects of two pollen substitutes on brood mortality and length of adult life in the honeybee. J. Apic. Res. 22: 49-52.

Wolff, C. M. 1968. Principles of biometry. Van Nostrand, Princeton, N.J.